Late-onset blindness linked to enhanced emotional speech processing

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

A recent study published in the journal Cognition and Emotion has revealed that blindness, whether present from birth or acquired later in life, does not impede the ability to process emotional speech. Interestingly, the study found that individuals who became blind later in life might have enhanced abilities in this area compared to sighted individuals.

Emotional speech processing involves recognizing and interpreting emotions conveyed through speech, which is essential for navigating social interactions. Previous research has provided conflicting results on whether blindness impairs or enhances this ability.

Some studies suggested that the lack of early visual experiences might hinder emotional speech processing, while others highlighted the brain’s remarkable ability to adapt and compensate for sensory deficits. The new study sought to resolve these contradictions by examining the role of visual experience in emotional speech processing among individuals with congenital blindness, late-onset blindness, and sighted controls.

“In our team, we combine different expertise: communication disorders (Dr. Icht), blindness (Dr. Chebat) and spoken emotion processing (me),” said study author Boaz M. Ben-David, the chair of the Communication, Aging, and Neuropsychology Lab (CANlab) at the Baruch Ivcher School of Psychology at Reichman University.

“We created this collaboration to better understand how late and early sensory deprivation affects emotional processing. More generally, studying individuals with late and congenital blindness allows us to explore the impact of early visual experience on cognitive functions, particularly in how emotional information is processed through auditory channels. This research aimed not only to deepen our knowledge of brain plasticity, but also to have practical implications for developing better communication strategies for visually impaired individuals.”

The researchers recruited 53 participants, divided into three groups: 17 individuals with congenital blindness, 15 with late-onset blindness, and 21 sighted individuals. The participants were tested using the Hebrew version of the Test of Rating of Emotions in Speech (T-RES), which assesses the ability to identify and selectively attend to emotional content in speech.

The T-RES presented spoken sentences with different combinations of emotional content in two channels: semantics (the meaning of words) and prosody (intonation). The sentences could be congruent, with matching emotional content in both channels, or incongruent, with different emotions in each channel.

Participants were asked to rate the extent to which they agreed that the speaker conveyed a predefined emotion (anger, happiness, or sadness) using a six-point scale. The study consisted of two tasks: prosodic-rating, focusing on intonation, and semantic-rating, focusing on word meaning. The tasks were counterbalanced to control for order effects.

Participants also completed the Digit Span Test, which assesses working memory by having participants recall sequences of numbers presented audibly. This test was included to explore the role of cognitive resources in processing emotional speech.

The researchers found that individuals with late-onset blindness demonstrated superior abilities in identifying emotions conveyed through both prosody and semantics compared to both sighted individuals and those with congenital blindness. This group showed higher accuracy in detecting emotional content, suggesting that early visual experiences might enhance the ability to process emotional speech later in life.

In terms of selective attention, the late-onset blindness group also outperformed the other groups. They were better able to focus on one channel (either prosody or semantics) while ignoring the other, indicating a heightened ability to filter and attend to relevant emotional information in speech. This suggests that individuals who lost their sight later in life have developed compensatory mechanisms that enhance their emotional speech processing abilities.

“One surprising finding was that individuals with late blindness outperformed sighted individuals in tasks involving the identification and selective attention to emotional speech,” Ben-David told PsyPost. “This suggests that early visual experiences, even if lost later in life, play a significant role in enhancing spoken emotion processing abilities.”

Interestingly, there were no significant differences between the congenital blindness group and the sighted control group in either emotional identification or selective attention. This finding suggests that congenital blindness does not impair the ability to process emotional speech. It highlights the remarkable adaptability of the human brain, which can compensate for the lack of visual experiences through other sensory modalities and cognitive mechanisms.

Another crucial finding was the role of working memory in moderating these abilities. Participants with higher working memory scores, as measured by the Digit Span Test, performed better in emotional speech processing tasks. This indicates that cognitive resources play a significant role in compensating for the absence of early visual experiences. In particular, the advantage of individuals with late-onset blindness over those with congenital blindness was more pronounced in participants with lower working memory capacity.

“The average person should take away that blindness, whether congenital or late-onset, does not hinder the ability to process emotional speech,” Ben-David explained. “In fact, individuals with late blindness may even have enhanced abilities compared to sighted individuals. This suggests that the brain can compensate for the loss of one sense by strengthening others, highlighting the remarkable adaptability of human cognition.”

“Another important take home message is the importance of early visual exposure for the development of spoken-emotion processing skills. Although spoken emotions are heard rather than seen, the all-important multi-sensory experience in the early years plays an important role. Finally, the moderating effect of cognitive abilities (working memory), indicates their importance in everyday life.”

But the study, like all research, includes some limitations. The sample size was relatively small, and the participants were all native Hebrew speakers, limiting the generalizability of the findings to other languages and cultures. Additionally, the study used a single professional female actress to deliver the speech stimuli, which might not represent the full range of emotional expressions found in natural speech. Future research should include larger and more diverse samples to validate these findings across different languages and cultural contexts.

Another area for future research is to explore the impact of other individual differences, such as personality traits and cognitive styles, on emotional speech processing in blindness. Understanding how these factors interact with sensory experiences could help develop targeted interventions to improve social communication skills in individuals with visual impairments.

“Our long-term goals are to further investigate the mechanisms of sensory compensation and brain plasticity in various sensory-deprived and cognitively-challenged populations, across the lifespan,” Ben-David said. “We aim to explore how different compensation mechanisms are developed and activated. We hope to develop interventions that leverage these compensatory mechanisms to improve cognition. In addition, we seek to understand how differences in emotional processing alter our shared reality, in order to improve communication between groups.”

“I’d like to add that our findings emphasize the importance of carefully investigating emotion processing. It appears that this topic is sometimes considered as ‘not serious enough,’ but I urge you to ‘get emotional’ about it. As well as advancing scientific knowledge, this line of research has practical implications for enhancing social interaction and communication across populations.”

“The more tools we use and the more diverse populations we test, the more we are able to learn about how processing emotions in communication reflects the basic cognitive processes,” Ben-David explained. “More importantly, communication is the core of human experience. Let’s gain a deeper understanding of how it is shaped.”

The study, “‘Love looks not with the eyes’: supranormal processing of emotional speech in individuals with late-blindness versus preserved processing in individuals with congenital-blindness,” was authored by Boaz M. Ben-David, Daniel-Robert Chebat, and Michal Icht.

© PsyPost